Introducing Logophoricity to the Indonesian Grammar
18/07/2020 Views : 709
I Nyoman Udayana
In this article I will talk about logophoricity in Indonesian. However, before we begin with the account of this inquiry. Pertaining to the title of this article; a question can be raised as to why logophoricity is introduced to the Indonesian grammar. Do not we have this notion at all in Indonesian? We come to the answer to the question shortly. First of all, when we talk about the Indonesian grammar, because this notion logophoricity has to do with the realm of pronouns, we need to start out with the concept of personal pronouns in Indonesian.
We are familiar with the universal classification of pronouns such as personal pronouns namely saya ‘first singular person pronoun I’, kamu second person pronoun ‘you’, dia ‘third singular personal pronoun she/ he’, kami ‘we’ which is first plural personal pronoun inclusive and the pronoun we can be equivalent with kita in Indonesian which is first plural personal pronoun exclusive, and lastly mereka third plural personal pronoun they’. In addition to this, we might be also familiar with what is called reflexive pronoun. A reflexive pronoun is a pronoun that operates in a sentential level of a grammar of any natural language in which the subject of a verbal predicate denoting an action or a psychological state is the same as the object of that action/state named by the predicate. In other words, the actor and the undergoer are realized by the same entity, one entity is the mirror image of the other. Indonesian itself does not have a dedicated reflexive pronoun but instead it is derived from the word meaning ‘body’, diri. The reflexive pronouns in Indonesian are divided into two categories, simple reflexive and complex reflexives. Simple reflexive is realized by diri itself while the complex reflexive is formed by the combination of the simple reflexive with possessive pronouns thus deriving reflexives such as diri saya ‘myself’’, dirimu ‘yourself’, dirinya ‘himself/herself/themselves’, diri kami/diri kita ‘ourselves’. In terms of the syntactic distribution, the simple reflexive pronoun occurs with action verbs while the complex reflexive pronouns with non-action verbs. Each of them is illustrated in the following examples.
(1) Action verbs:
a. John memukul diri
John Act.hit self
‘John hit himself’
b. Jane mencubit diri
Jane Act.pinch self
‘Jane pinched himself’
(2) Non-action verbs
a. Saya melihat diri saya di kaca
1SG Act.see self.1SGPOSS in mirror
‘I saw myself in the mirror’
b. Kami mempercayai diri kami
1PL Act believe. Self 1PLPOSS
‘We believe in ourselves’
c. Dia/mereka menyintai dirinya
3SG/3PL Act.love self.3POSS
‘She/he loves herself/himself/ they love themselves’
What is worthy of a note here is that the complex reflexive in which the possessive morpheme attached to diri serves as a modifier which only strictly co-occurs with non action verbs. An attempt to make the reflexive anaphor realized by the complex reflexive in action verbs result in the fact that the modifier is optional. On the other hand, to make simple reflexive co-occur with the non-action verbs yields an ungrammatical construction (marked by an asterisk *). Thus each of the sentences in (1) and (2) above can be rewritten as (3) and (4) respectively.
(3) Action verbs:
a. John memukul diri-(nya)
John AV.hit self-3POSS
‘John hit himself’
b. Jane mencubit diri-(nya)
Jane AV.pinch self-3POSS
‘Jane pinched herself’
(4) Non-action verbs
a. Saya melihat diri saya/ *diri di kaca
1SG AV.see self 1SGPOSS self in mirror
‘I saw myself in the mirror’
b. Kami mempercayai diri kami/ *diri
1PL AV.believe. self 1PLPOSS self
‘We believe in ourselves’
c. Dia/ mereka menyintai dirinya/ *diri
3SG/ 3PL AV.love self.3POSS self
‘She/he loves herself/himself/ they love themselves’
Logophoricity and its domain
The concept of logophoricity is first introduced to the linguistic literature by the seminal work of Hagège (1974) when he accounted for the distribution of the pronoun in African languages. Logophoricity is a linguistic system in which logophoric pronouns are found in the clausal complement of a predicate (Hagège 1974). They refer to the persons whose words, thoughts or emotion are represented. The term logophoric pronouns are common in West African languages, such as Ewe and Gokana. The nature of how logophoricity is encoded may vary from language to language. In Ewe, logophoric pronoun and regular pronoun are encoded by different forms while, in Gokana, logophoricity is marked on the verb as shown in the following examples.
Ewe (Clements 1975: 142):
(5) a. Kofi be yè-dzo.
Kofi say Log-leave
‘Kofii said that hei left.’
b. Kofi be e-dzo.
Kofi say Pro-leave
‘Kofii said that hej left.’
Gokana (Hyman and Comrie 1981: 20):
(6) a. aè kɔ aè dɔ̀.
Pro said Pro fell
‘Hei said hej fell.’
b. aè kɔ aè dɔ̀-ɛ̀.
Pro said Pro fell-Log
‘Hei said that hei fell.’
Languages found outside West Africa such as Icelandic and Japanese use reflexive anaphor for encoding logophoricity. This is illustrated respectively in (7) and (8).
Icelandic (Sigurðson 1986, cited in Sells 1987: 450):
(7) a. Hanni sagði [að sigi vantaði hæfileika].
hei said [that selfi lacked ability]
‘Hei said that hei lacked ability.’
b. *Honumi var sagt [að sigi vantaði hæfileika].
hei was told [that selfi lacked ability]
‘Hei was told that hei lacked ability.’
Japanese (Kameyama 1985, cited in Sells 1987: 453-454):
(8) a. Takasii wa Taroo ni [Yosiko ga zibuni o
Takasii Top Taroo Dat [Yosiko Subj selfi Obj
nikundeiru koto] o hanasita.
be-hating Comp] Obj told
‘Takasii told Taroo that Yosiko hated himi.’
As noted, Indonesian does not have logophoric pronouns per se. However like all languages outside West Africa such as Japanese and Icelandic as illustrated in (7) and 8) (see also Huang 2000), Indonesian also uses reflexive pronouns to function as logophoric pronouns. The reflexive pronouns used are limited to complex reflexive pronouns. Thus the same form is used logophorically. Unlike Icelandic and Japanese, Indonesian never uses simplex reflexive anaphors to express logophoricity. However, needless to say, the distribution of logophoric pronouns in all of these languages (Icelandic, Japanese, and Indonesian) is completely the same. As has been explained, the reflexive pronouns are used in a sentence/clause in which the antecedent and the reflexive pronoun are co-argument of a verbal predicate but logophoric use of the reflexive pronouns have a distribution which accords with the use of dedicated logophoric pronouns found in West African languages. In other words, the logophoric use of complex reflexives operates in a domain where the antecedent of a logophoric pronoun contains in the matrix clause while the logophor is found in the clausal argument of the matrix predicate. Thus I simply call the complex reflexives used in the logophoric environment logophoric pronouns in Indonesian. In Sneddon et al. (2010), Indonesian reflexivization is well discussed. However, the logophoric system is not touched on at all. Thus, although the two systems, reflexivization and logophoricity, depart from the same forms, given their distinct distribution and their respective importance in the linguistic operation, the logophoric system needs to be introduced or accounted for in the Indonesian grammar.
Verbs that license logophoricity
When we talk about reflexivization, the verbs used for constructing it can be said to be unrestricted provided that the verb in question allows the antecedent-(reflexive) anaphor relations and such a verb must be transitive verbs (verbs that require the occurrence/presence of an object). However, verbs that form a logophoric domain are limited to verbs of communication, verbs of perception, and verbs of mental state. The examples are in order.
Verbs of communication
(9) a. Diai mengatakan bahwa dirinyai akan datang
3SG AV.say COMP self.3POSS AUX come
‘(S)he said that (s)he would come’
b. Tonoi melaporkan bahwa John telah membohongi dirinyai
Tono AV.report COMP John PERF AV.deceive self.3POSS
‘Tono reported that john had deceived him’
Verbs of perception
(10) a. Tinii mendengar bahwa Ali mencintai dirinyai
Tini AV.hear COMP Ali AV.love self.3POSS
‘Tini heard that Ali loved her’
b. Tonoi merasa bahwa John telah mengikuti dirinyai
Tono AV.feel COMP John PERF AV.follow self.3POSS
‘Tono felt that John was following him’
Verbs of mental state
(11) a. Amini percaya bahwa Ali membenci dirinyai
Amin believe COMP Ali AV.hate self.3POSS
‘Amin believes that Ali hates him’
b. Diai tahu bahwa John menunggu dirinyai
3SG know COMP John AV.wait.for self.3POSS
‘(S)he knew that John was waiting for him/her
References
Clements, George N. 1975. The logophoric pronoun in Ewe: Its role in discourse. Journal of West African Languages 10.141-77
Hagège, Claude. 1974. Les pronoms logophoriques. Bulletin de la société de linguistique de Paris 69.287-310
Hyman, Larry M. and Bernard Comrie. 1981. Logophoric reference in Gokana. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 3.19-37.
Huang, Yan. 2000. Anaphora: A cross-linguistic study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sells, Peter. 1987. Aspects of logophoricity. Linguistic Inquiry 18.445-479.
Sneddon, James Neil, Alexander Adelaar, Dwi Noverni Djenar, and Michael C. Ewing. 2010. Indonesian: A comprehensive grammar. London: Routledge